top of page
Search

Application of Prospect Theory in COVID-19 Vaccine Intentions



Introduction

  • Would gain and loss frames have effectively improved COVID-19 vaccine intentions during the COVID-19 pandemic?

  • Prospect Theory can be applied to improve vaccination campaigns by understanding that:

  • People evaluate vaccination decisions based on perceived gains and losses rather than absolute outcomes.

  • Message effectiveness varies based on individual factors - hesitant people respond better to loss frames while those at high perceived risk prefer gain frames.

  • Cultural differences matter - individualistic societies respond to personal benefit messaging while collectivist cultures favour community-focused approaches.

  • Tailored communication strategies based on these insights can significantly improve public health outcomes.


Key Ideas


Understanding Prospect Theory in the Context of Vaccination

  • Prospect Theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979) suggests that people evaluate decisions based on perceived gains and losses rather than absolute outcomes.

  • The framing of information influences behaviour:

    • Gain frames emphasise the benefits of action (e.g., “Getting vaccinated will protect you and your loved ones”).

    • Loss frames highlight risks of inaction (e.g., “Not getting vaccinated increases your risk of severe illness and death”).

  • In healthcare, people are generally risk-averse when faced with potential gains and risk-seeking when faced with potential losses.

  • Applying this to vaccine hesitancy:

    • If vaccination is seen as low risk with high benefits, people prefer gain-framed messages.

    • If vaccination is seen as risky with uncertain benefits, loss-framed messages may be more effective.


Baseline Vaccine Intentions and Framing Effects

  • Prospect Theory predicts that framing effects depend on prior attitudes toward vaccines.

  • Studies show that hesitant individuals (those with low baseline vaccine intentions) are more likely to be influenced by loss frames.

    • Example: A study in Pennsylvania healthcare workers found that loss-framed messages doubled vaccine registration rates compared to the control group.

      • The fear of missing out on protection or experiencing severe illness drove behaviour change.

  • Conversely, people with high vaccine intentions showed little change in response to either frame.

    • Example: A Swedish study found that gain and loss frames had no significant impact on individuals already willing to vaccinate.

      • These individuals had already decided, so framing had little influence.


Perceived Risk & Severity of COVID-19

  • People assess risk differently based on their perception of the risk of getting infected and the severity of the symptoms of COVID-19.

  • Low perceived risk of COVID-19 → Loss frames more effective

    • Younger adults often viewed COVID-19 as a mild illness, making them less likely to vaccinate.

      • A Japanese study found that loss-framed messages emphasising the societal impact of low vaccination rates increased vaccine intentions among young adults.

  • High perceived risk of COVID-19 → Gain frames more effective

    • Older adults and people with pre-existing conditions saw themselves as more vulnerable.

      • A study found that gain-framed messages reinforcing personal health benefits led to greater willingness to vaccinate among older adults (e.g. reducing severe illness and death).


Cultural Influence: Collectivism vs Individualism

  • Individualistic cultures (USA, UK, Netherlands) → More responsive to individual gain/loss framing (e.g., “Protect yourself”).

  • Collectivist cultures (Japan) → More responsive to collective gain/loss framing (e.g., “Protect your community”).

  • Example: A US study found that people with conservative ideologies responded better to individual-focused messages, while liberal individuals were more influenced by collective messages.



Limitations

  • Vaccination is a high-risk preventive behaviour, unlike wearing a seatbelt or exercising.

    • There are 2 main health-related behaviours: detective and preventive.

      • Detective is usually at higher risk than preventive behaviours.

    • This unique nature makes it more difficult to make accurate predictions according to the general concepts in Prospect Theory and framing

  • Fear-based messaging (loss frames) can backfire, increasing resistance due to psychological reactance (a defensive reaction to perceived coercion).

  • All of my research used participants' self-reported vaccine intentions as the outcome, but in reality that doesn't always translate into actual vaccine uptake.

  • Other factors influence vaccine uptake, such as misinformation, access to vaccines, and distrust in government or pharmaceutical companies.



Conclusion: Would gain/loss frames have been effective?

  • Partially 

  • Loss frames were effective for hesitant individuals and those who felt low risk from COVID-19.

  • Gain frames worked better for those who perceived high risk from COVID-19.

  • However, other psychological and social factors played a role, suggesting that framing alone is not enough to overcome vaccine hesitancy.

  • For future vaccine campaigns, tailoring messages to different populations (based on risk perception and cultural values) would maximise effectiveness.




Comments


bottom of page